1
|
|
2
|
|
3
|
|
4
|
- The study of material culture and cultural institutions in a comparative
frame can contribute to prehistory in the same way as genetics and
linguistics.
- Many types of material culture
inevitably have a very limited presence in the archaeological record;
nevertheless there is every reason to consider they were integral to the
societies whose lifeways we are trying to reconstruct.
- Musical instruments represent an interesting case, because their
morphology is so marked that they can be unambiguously identified.
- Where a particular language phylum which has a strong archaeological
correlates, such as Austronesian, it should be possible to piece
together the relations between the expansion of the phylum and its
changing musical culture.
|
5
|
- Material culture studies tend to be synchronic in nature; more attention
is given to the use of tin cans in society than to the historical
process where such cans came to be diffused worldwide in a short period
of time. In more recent times, the spread of material culture has been
completely delinked from demographic movement through the operations of
multi-national businesses.
- But in the recent past, the establishment of the great language phyla of
the world was in part associated with movement of populations and in
turn the spread of languages and associated cultural elements.
- The assumption that this was the case was hot news in 1900 and up to the
1930s. German and Swedish schools of material culture studies, Kulturkreislehre
et al., actively trawled
ethnographic and museum material to produce distribution maps.
|
6
|
- Within the Anglo-American tradition such studies began to die during the
1930s, and part of the reason was that no effective explanatory models
underlay these vast assemblages of data.
- But from the 1990s, the alliance of archaeology, genetics and
linguistics has provided just such a frame. If Austronesian or
Indo-European or Uto-Aztecan can be modelled as a historical expansion
of people and culture as well as language then the distribution of
material culture can be correlated with these other elements.
- It is important not to confuse this with conventional diffusionism,
which is typically concerned with ‘high-culture’ elements that are most
likely to have spread (or not) well after the establishment of the major
language phyla. The argument is that there are certain items of material
culture of sufficient antiquity that they were carried, literally, by
the movement of speakers as the phylum evolved.
|
7
|
- Typical objections to this procedure concern independent invention and
negative evidence and these can be very real objections with functional
technologies such as hunting techniques. But musical instruments have
essentially arbitrary morphologies, rather like lexemes represent
arbitrary choices of phonemes to refer to something
- It is easy to show that there is a low index of reinvention and that
numerous geographical bottlenecks exist, that once lost, an instrument
does not tend to be reinvented, as Australia and the New World
illustrate
- Two previous papers have looked at the way musical instruments can be
used as markers for other types of reconstruction of prehistory. At
EURASEAA I presented a reconstruction of musical practices in the
Austronesian region that could be traced back to Taiwan. This a
follow-up, describing instruments that appear to be typical of the
Austronesian region but which are not attested in Taiwan.
|
8
|
|
9
|
|
10
|
|
11
|
|
12
|
|
13
|
- Instruments include the leg-xylophone, multi-blade Jews’ harps, stamping
tubes, musical bow and small struck bamboo slit-gong
- Musical practices include polyphonic choral singing, the stick dance.
- Many of these are widespread in the Austronesian area
- But a whole suite of instruments appear to be developed in the
Philippines and to spread out from there
|
14
|
- A highly distinctive feature of much of the Oceanian culture area is
shark-calling. Individuals or groups are deemed to have particular
relations with sharks and to be able to call them by means of the
shark-sistrum. These are cane-loops with rattling materials threaded on
the loop and are held in the hand and rattled (underwater?). These are
recorded from the Sulu region, Biak, New Britain, New Ireland and Samoa
and probably exist in intermediate areas (Sachs 1928; Collaer 1965:78;
Kunst 1967).
|
15
|
- The distribution of the concussion-buzzer is very intriguing as it
occurs in Luzon, Nias, Sulawesi and among the Orang Asli in the Malay
Peninsula. Although this is typical of the Austronesian region, it seems
difficult to explain why it should have disappeared from so much of the
central area of the Philippines and somehow bypassed Borneo.
|
16
|
- Despite their pan-western Austronesian distribution, gongs are almost
certainly not present in the first wave of Austronesian expansion. On
chronological grounds, the type of bronze technology required to make
them had not been developed by the putative period of early expansion
(3800 BP). It seems likely that they were developed and spread on the
mainland and made their way gradually eastwards to the islands in a
series of trade flows. The development of tuned sets of gongs may be a
typical Javanese development that spread back the mainland.
|
17
|
|
18
|
- Drums were unknown in Taiwan so they seem to have been innovated in the
post-Formosan region. Although drums are very common and clearly ancient
in mainland cultures, a particular type of drum evolved in the
Austronesian region, the wedge-laced drum
- Drums with similar systems of tensioning the head occur in the
Philippines, Sulawesi, Borneo, and much of Indonesia. The furthest
extent seems to be the Bird’s Head Peninsula in West Papua
- the term kimbal in Ibaloi in Luzon is clearly cognate with gimbal (Batak
in Palawan) and Mamanwa gimbar in Mindanao. Both the drum and the name
can be reconstructed with some confidence to proto-Philippines.
|
19
|
- The gourd-resonated musical bow, where a dried gourd, coconut-shell or
tin can is attached to the bow and the string struck with sticks. The
resonator is open at the base and placed against the chest, thus
muffling or amplifying the sound. This instrument is recorded from Luzon
in the Northern Philippines and nowhere else in the Austronesian region
except on the island of Guam in the Marianas, which seems to correlate
rather well with archaeological and linguistic findings
|
20
|
|
21
|
- The idiochord tube-zither is a bamboo internode with strings raised up
from the epidermis (hence the term idiochord) between two internodes.
Small wooden bridges are inserted under the strings both to lift them
from the surface of the bamboo and to tune them. All these are
relatively recent and the basic type is still widely played in the
Philippines and Indonesia.
- The broad distribution of the tube-zither is Philippines, Indonesia,
Madagascar, Việt Nam. The Aslian groups of the Malay peninsula
also play the zither, although not the surrounding Malay. Its presence
in Madagascar is presumably due to the migrations of the Maanyan, one of
the Barito groups now thought to have been the main source of the
peopling of the Great Isle. Tube-zithers are entirely absent in
Melanesia and Polynesia, and
Oceanic speakers had already lost his instrument when they began
their expansion.
|
22
|
|
23
|
- The Seychelles are not usually considered part of the Austronesian zone,
but a curious piece of evidence suggests a link, probably with
Madagascar, perhaps through the slave trade. A remarkable Seychellois
tube-zither, one of the disappearing anciens instruments, which is now
used as a type of end-blown horn. The instrument retains the
organological properties of the Austronesian tube-zither but these are
no longer understood by the players.
|
24
|
- This instrument is made in a similar fashion to the polychordal
tube-zither, but it has only one or two strings, supported by a common
bridge, and is not intended to be tuned. The noise is typically an
indeterminate buzzing sound. The strings are either plucked with the
fingers or beaten with two light sticks, rather like a slit-gong. In the
Philippines at least, these instruments are used to scare birds away
from the corn. They occur through most of Indonesia including Sumatra,
Nias and Sulawesi, but are only recorded in Melanesia from Biak in a
monochord form and then disappear altogether
|
25
|
|
26
|
|
27
|
- The stick-zither is a flat stick with projections on one end, and one or
two strings are stretched along its length. The strings are strummed and
then the projections used like frets to alter the pitch of the string.
The stick-zither is unknown in the Philippines, but widespread in
Borneo, Sulawesi and was also carried to Madagascar and the East African
coast. The exact distribution is still poorly recorded, but it is
present on Ternate, Halmahera and Sumba, but absent from the main
islands of Indonesia
|
28
|
- The notch-flute is a hollow, cylindrical flute made from a reed with a
V-shaped notch cut in the end which acts as an embouchure. Notch-flutes
are found virtually throughout the Philippines, in Indonesia. It seems
to be absent in New Guinea. The notch-flute is one of the few
instruments where a linguistic reconstruction is possible; from Isneg in
Luzon to Mamanwa in Mindanao, the name is *p-l-n-d-g, suggesting that
the instrument was carried throughout the region
|
29
|
|
30
|
- Panpipes are single-note tubes, closed at the base, bound together in
sets. Typically in this region, there are four or five tubes and they
are relatively short. Panpipes are found in the Cordillera or Northern
Luzon. Panpipes are apparently absent in the rest of the Philippines and
in Sulawesi although they are recorded in Java, but common in parts of
New Guinea and the Solomons. In New Guinea, panpipes occur in the Sepik
river area and on New Ireland. The panpipes of the Are’are of the
Solomons have been extensively recorded and described by Hugo Zemp. For
Polynesia it seems that only museum specimens exist for Samoa (Moyle
1988:51) and Tonga
- This gap in distribution leads to the question as to whether these
occurrences of panpipes are connected. Between the Cordillera and the
Sepik is a long distance for panpipes to be completely eliminated.
Moreover, the Melanesian region has a whole spectrum of panpipe types.
However, panpipes were recorded both in Fiji and in most of the
Polynesian islands even as far as New Zealand, although they are
moribund throughout this region. It may thus be that the early
Austronesian panpipes are connected with South China (and the culture of
rice-terraces) and that the panpipes of Melanesia are part of the
ancient Papuan culture. Oceanic speakers would then have picked them up
in the encounter with Papuans and they would have spread in turn to Fiji
and Polynesia.
|
31
|
|
32
|
- The instruments discussed here are typical of Austronesian-speakers with
one important exception, the Aslian or aboriginal populations of the
Malay peninsula
- The Aslian speak Austroasiatic languages, but these instruments are not
found elsewhere in the Austroasiatic world
- It is therefore likely they took them over from the pre-Malay, diverse
Austronesians who once inhabited the Malay peninsula. As the Malay
expanded, adopted Islam and a musical culture influenced by Java, only
the Aslian maintained the prior musical culture of the region
|
33
|
- Austronesian expanded rapidly across lightly inhabited territory and
largely carried with it a repertoire of material culture.
- Human inventiveness is quite limited in the sphere of musical
instruments, hence we can associate particular types with specific
language groups, especially in this region.
- Distributional data also enables us to detect which instruments, now apparently typical of
the region, represent later spread and are therefore not associated with
the initial expansion.
|
34
|
- Clearly this method can be applied to other spheres of synchronic
material culture. It enables us to reconstruct aspects of culture that
would be invisible archaeologically and sometimes linguistically.
- But this type of study is virtually dead, which is unfortunate, as
current material culture is being eliminated rapidly without being
documented
|
35
|
|
36
|
|